Conspiracy theories can form a monological belief system: A self-sustaining worldview comprised of a network of mutually supportive beliefs. The present research shows that even mutually incompatible conspiracy theories are positively correlated in endorsement. In Study 1 (n = 137), the more participants believed that Princess Diana faked her own death, the more they believed that she was murdered. In Study 2 (n = 102), the more participants believed that Osama Bin Laden was already dead when U.S. special forces raided his compound in Pakistan, the more they believed he is still alive. Hierarchical regression models showed that mutually incompatible conspiracy theories are positively associated because both are associated with the view that the authorities are engaged in a cover-up (Study 2). The monological nature of conspiracy belief appears to be driven not by conspiracy theories directly supporting one another but by broader beliefs supporting conspiracy theories in general.

. Correlations between endorsement of Princess Diana conspiracy theories in Study 1.

Figures - uploaded by Karen M. Douglas

Author content

All figure content in this area was uploaded by Karen M. Douglas

Content may be subject to copyright.

ResearchGate Logo

Discover the world's research

  • 20+ million members
  • 135+ million publications
  • 700k+ research projects

Join for free

Uncorrected manuscript

Dead and Alive: Beliefs in Contradictory Conspiracy Theories

Michael J. Wood, Karen M. Douglas & Robbie M. Sutton,

University of Kent, Canterbury, United Kingdom

Keywords: conspiracy theories, conspiracism, contradiction, explanatory coherence

CORRESPONDING AUTHOR:

Michael J. Wood

University of Kent

School of Psychology – Keynes College, University of Kent

Canterbury, Kent, CT2 7NP, United Kingdom

mw337@kent.ac.uk

AUTHOR BIOS:

Michael J. Wood is a Ph.D. candidate at the University of Kent, United Kingdom. His research

focuses on the psychology of conspiracy theories.

Karen M. Douglas is a Reader in Psychology at the University of Kent, United Kingdom. She

has published widely on topics such as language and stereotyping, persuasion, the psychology of

the internet and the psychology of conspiracy theories.

Robbie M. Sutton is a Reader in Psychology at the University of Kent, United Kingdom. His

research interests include the psychology of justice, gender, and intergroup processes.

DECLARATION OF CONFLICT OF INTEREST:

The authors declared no potential conflicts of interests with respect to the authorship and/or

publication of this article.

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE/FUNDING:

The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research and/or

authorship of this article: School of Psychology, University of Kent.

Dead and Alive: Beliefs in Contradictory Conspiracy Theories

Abstract:

Conspiracy theories can form a monological belief system: a self-sustaining worldview

comprised of a network of mutually supportive beliefs. The present research shows that even

endorsement of mutually incompatible conspiracy theories are positively correlated. In Study 1

(n = 137), the more participants believed that Princess Diana faked her own death, the more they

believed that she was murdered. In Study 2 (n = 102), the more participants believed that Osama

Bin Laden was already dead when U.S. special forces raided his compound in Pakistan, the more

they believed he is still alive. Hierarchical regression models showed that mutually incompatible

conspiracy theories are positively associated because both are associated with the view that the

authorities are engaged in a cover-up (Study 2). The monological nature of conspiracy belief

appears to be driven not by conspiracy theories directly supporting one another, but by broader

beliefs supporting conspiracy theories in general.

Article text:

A conspiracy theory is defined as a proposed plot by powerful people or organizations

working together in secret to accomplish some (usually sinister) goal (Coady, 2006; Douglas &

Sutton, 2008; Goertzel, 1994). Popular contemporary examples include the theory that the 9/11

attacks were planned and carried out by elements within the American government (Kay, 2011)

and the belief that evidence of a causal link between autism and childhood vaccination is being

suppressed by an unscrupulous medical industry (Goertzel, 2010). Conspiracy theories are not

by definition false; indeed, many real conspiracies have come to light over the years. Suspicions

of President Nixon's involvement in a burglary at the headquarters of the Democratic National

Committee began as a seemingly outlandish conspiracy theory, but turned out to be true (Bale,

2007). However, conspiracy beliefs, even when wrong, are notoriously resistant to falsification,

and can take on the appearance of a "degenerating research program" (Clarke, 2002, p. 136),

with new layers of conspiracy being added to rationalize each new piece of disconfirming

evidence.

Spurred in part by the growth of new media, conspiracism has become a major subcultural

phenomenon. This shift has not gone unnoticed in academia. In recent decades there has been

an explosion of research into the psychology of belief in conspiracy theories. Much of this

research interest has focussed on the individual correlates of conspiracy belief, but perhaps the

most consistent finding in the work on the psychology of conspiracy theories is that belief in a

particular theory is strongly predicted by belief in others – even ostensibly unrelated ones

(Douglas & Sutton, 2008; Goertzel, 1994 ; Swami, Chamorro-Premuzic, & Furnham, 2010;

Swami et al., 2011). For instance, someone who believes that the American government was

behind the 9/11 attacks is very likely to also believe that Princess Diana was deliberately

assassinated. One proposed explanation for this connection is that beliefs in conspiracy theories

somehow support one another (Goertzel, 1994). Even though the perpetrators may be different

in each case, the fact that one massive, sinister conspiracy could be successfully executed in

near-perfect secrecy suggests that many such plots are possible. Over time, the view of the

world as a place ruled by conspiracies can lead to conspiracy becoming the default explanation

for any given event – a unitary, closed-off worldview in which beliefs come together in a

mutually supportive network known as a monological belief system (Goertzel, 1994; Clarke,

2002; Swami et al., 2010; Swami et al., 2011).

However, some conspiracy theories emphatically do not support one another; indeed, many

provide mutually contradictory explanations for the same event. These contradictions among

conspiracy theories are the focus of the present article. For instance, the theories surrounding the

death of Princess Diana vary widely; some claim that she was killed by MI6, others allege that

she was killed by Mohammed al-Fayed's business enemies, still others that she faked her own

death. How does a conspiracy-believing observer reconcile the presence of these competing,

mutually contradictory accounts? If beliefs in conspiracy theories are correlated with one

another because the theories are in direct agreement, one would not expect reliable correlations

between beliefs in theories that are mutually exclusive.

In the present research, we seek to determine whether the coherence of the conspiracist

belief system is driven not by direct relationships among individual theories, but by agreement

between individual theories and higher-order beliefs about the world. For instance, the idea that

authorities are engaged in motivated deception of the public would be a cornerstone of

conspiracist thinking due to its centrality in conspiracy theories. Someone who believes in a

significant number of conspiracy theories would naturally begin to see authorities as

fundamentally deceptive, and new conspiracy theories would seem more plausible in light of that

belief (Read, Snow and Simon, 2003; Simon, Snow, & Read, 2004). Indeed, the two conspiracy

theories mentioned above – an autism/vaccine connection and 9/11 as an inside job – both

revolve around that central proposition. Likewise, whether one believes that Princess Diana was

killed by MI6 or Mohammed Al-Fayed's business enemies, belief in a cover-up would support

(and be supported by) both theories. In spite of that, the two theories contradict each other.

Would it be possible for their contradiction to be overruled by their coherence with a broader

conspiracist worldview, such that they display a positive correlation in endorsement?

Some literature on stereotyping suggests that coherence with strongly held worldviews

may well be sufficient to overwhelm contradictions between individual beliefs. Adorno (1954)

found strong positive correlations in endorsement between contradictory negative stereotypes of

Jews, such that highly prejudiced participants found them to be both too isolated from the rest of

society and too eager to participate in it. Adorno proposed that this paradoxical perception has

its roots in "a relatively blind hostility which is reflected in the stereotypy, self-contradiction, and

destructiveness" of anti-Jewish stereotyping (p. 76). In spite of their contradictory nature, both

stereotypes drew enough credibility from their one common element – a negative perception of

Jewish people – to end up with a strong positive association.

The same may well be true of

contradictory conspiracy theories; conspiracy advocates' distrust of official narratives may be so

strong that many alternative theories are simultaneously endorsed in spite of any contractions

between them.

The phenomenon of global coherence overruling local contradictions is perhaps best

understood in the context of Thagard's (1988) explanatory coherence model of social inference.

Explanatory coherence theory characterizes explanations and pieces of evidence about actors and

events as either coherent or incoherent with one another. These elements are represented by

nodes in a connectionist network. Activation flows from evidence nodes and higher-order

knowledge structures (Read, 1987) to the various explanations, which in turn excite or inhibit

one another depending on whether they are mutually coherent or contradictory. This process of

excitation and inhibition continues until the system reaches a stable equilibrium, at which point

the highly activated explanations are accepted and those with little activation are discarded.

Activation has been shown to flow the other way, as well: not only do evidence and higher-order

knowledge structures change one's perception of explanations, emerging conclusions in the

network also change perceptions of evidence and alter broad worldviews (Read & Miller, 1993;

Read et al., 2003).

For instance, imagine that someone is heavily invested in conspiracism and strongly

believes in a wide variety of different conspiracy theories. A view of authority as fundamentally

deceptive is coherent with all of these theories, and as such draws activation from them until it

becomes a strongly held belief in itself. When a novel conspiracy theory is presented, it

immediately seems more credible because it agrees with this now strongly held view and

disagrees with the officially endorsed narrative. Such higher-order beliefs may be so strongly

held that any conspiracy theory that stands in opposition to the official narrative will gain some

degree of endorsement from someone who holds a conspiracist worldview, even if it directly

contradicts other conspiracy theories that they also find credible. In other words, a natural

consequence of the explanatory coherence approach to social explanation is an instantiation of

the principle "the enemy of my enemy is my friend."

Indeed, this is a principle found explicitly in Heider's (1958) theory of psychological

balance, which shares a considerable common ground with explanatory coherence. In balance

theory, perceptions of an object or social actor are affected by its relationship with other actors

about which opinions already exist. For instance, people's evaluations of a novel product

endorsed by a known celebrity are more positive if they view the celebrity positively, or more

negative if their views of the celebrity are negative. In the case of conspiracy theories, we

propose that a similar mechanism is at work: officials are seen as deceptive, perhaps even

actively malevolent, so any explanation that they endorse is at a disadvantage, while alternative

explanations are more credible from the start. Explanatory coherence has been shown to

naturally instantiate many of the Gestalt principles on which balance theory is based (Read et al.,

2003), and others have noted the applicability of balance theory to the study of conspiracy belief,

such as Inglehart (1987)

.

Thus, we predict that for someone with a conspiracist worldview, nearly any theory that

assumes deception by officialdom in its explanation for a world event and stands in opposition to

the "mainstream" account will garner some agreement. This relationship may hold even to the

point that people who believe in a world governed by conspiracy are likely to endorse

contradictory conspiracy theories about the same topic. Just as Adorno (1954) found positive

correlations in endorsement of contradictory stereotypes, we expect to see positive relationships

between endorsement of contradictory conspiracy theories about the same event. For example,

the more that participants believe that a person at the centre of a death-related conspiracy theory,

such as Princess Diana or Osama Bin Laden, is still alive, the more they also tend to believe that

the same person was killed, so long as the alleged manner of death involves deception by

officialdom.

Study 1

We first elected to examine the relationship between contradictory conspiracy theories

regarding the same event by asking about several rival accounts of Princess Diana's death.

Method

Participants

One hundred thirty seven undergraduate psychology students (83% female, mean age 20.4)

were recruited from a second-year research methods class at a British university. Participation

was voluntary and no compensation was given.

Materials and Procedure

For the purposes of the present study, we used the conspiracy theory belief scale used by

Douglas & Sutton (2011). The questionnaire was 17 items long and used a 7-point Likert scale

(1 = "strongly disagree," 7 = "strongly agree,"), to ascertain participants' agreement with a

variety of different conspiracy theories. These included 9/11 as an inside job, global warming as

a hoax by scientists and politicians, and the idea of a fake moon landing. Crucially, there were

five items regarding the death of Princess Diana (Douglas & Sutton, 2008; 2011; α = .83):

1. One or more rogue "cells" in the British secret service constructed and carried out a plot

to kill Diana

2. There was an official campaign by MI6 to assassinate Diana, sanctioned by elements of

the establishment

3. Diana faked her own death so that she and Dodi could retreat into isolation

4. Business enemies of Dodi and his father Mohammed Al-Fayed assassinated Dodi, with

the death of Diana a cover-up for the operation

5. Diana had to be killed because the British government could not accept that the mother of

the future king was involved with a Muslim Arab

Not all of these items are mutually contradictory. Diana might conceivably have learned of

a plot to kill her and faked her own death in response, so #3 and #2 do not necessarily contradict

one another. #1 and #2 differ in the degree to which the operation to kill Diana was officially

sanctioned, and not all participants would necessarily pick up on that difference. Likewise, #5

indicates the existence of a plot to kill Diana but does not specify whether it was successful, so it

does not explicitly contradict any of the other theories. However, there are some unambiguous

contradictions. #1, #3, and #4 all propose different accounts of Diana's apparent death: either

she was killed by a rogue cell of the British secret service (#1) or by business rivals of the

Fayeds (#4), or she faked her own death (#3). These three theories are mutually incompatible,

and will be the focus of analysis in the present study.

Results & Discussion

We first performed an exploratory principal components analysis to investigate the factor

structure of the scale. Based on a scree plot, we extracted two unrotated factors which together

accounted for 46.9% of scale variance. All items had loadings of at least .35 on the first factor in

the unrotated solution, suggesting that it represents generic conspiracy belief; the second factor

drew loadings only from the five items concerning climate change conspiracy theories, and thus

appears to be related to beliefs in these conspiracies in particular.

In line with this factor structure, and with previous findings of high correlations among

beliefs in different conspiracy theories, the scale showed reasonable reliability (α = .78). Most of

the questions were significantly correlated with one another despite covering different topics; for

instance, a belief that a rogue cell of MI6 was responsible for Diana's death was correlated with

belief in theories that HIV was created in a laboratory (r = .39), that the moon landing was a

hoax (r = .34), and that governments are covering up the existence of aliens (r = .23) (all ps <

.01). In line with this general pattern, there was a network of significant positive relationships

among the majority of the Princess Diana conspiracy theories (see Table 1). People who

believed that Diana faked her own death were marginally more likely to also believe that she was

killed by a rogue cell of British Intelligence (r = .15, p = .075) and significantly more likely to

also believe that she was killed by business enemies of the Fayeds (r = .25, p = .003). Similarly,

participants who found it likely that the Fayeds' business rivals were responsible for the death of

Diana were highly likely to also blame a rogue cell (r = .61, p < .001).

As can be seen in Table 1, the correlations in endorsement with the idea that Diana faked

her own death appear much lower than the rest, to the point that the only non-significant

correlation involves that theory. We believe this to be due to a floor effect rather than any sort of

response to contradiction; endorsement of the faked-own-death theory was extremely low in this

sample, with a mean of only 1.52 on a 7-point scale. This level of endorsement was significantly

lower than that of the other theories, for which agreement ranged from 2.51 (business rivals) to

2.98 (rogue cell) (all ps < .001). As an alternative approach to the relationship between the

faked-death theory and the rogue cell theory, we dichotomized responses to the faked-death item,

comparing those who gave the lowest possible response with those who responded more

positively. In accordance with the general pattern of results, participants who strongly disagreed

with the faked-death theory showed a lower level of agreement with the rogue cell theory (M =

2.75) than those who responded otherwise (M = 3.47; t(134) = -2.56, p = .01).

In line with our hypothesis, the results show mostly clear positive correlations in

endorsement of contradictory conspiracy theories. Intuitively, this does not make sense. One

would think that there ought to be a negative correlation between beliefs in contradictory

accounts of events – the more one believes in a particular theory, the less likely rival theories

will seem. One possible alternative explanation for these results is acquiescence bias:

participants may have simply replied in the same way to every question, resulting in positive

correlations across the scale regardless of the questions' content. However, the scale included a

reverse-coded Diana conspiracy item which read, "The death of Princess Diana was an

accident." Contrary to the acquiescence hypothesis, this item was consistently negatively

correlated with the rest of the scale, most notably r = -.75 with the rogue-cell item and r = -.65

with the MI6 item (both ps < .001).

These results suggest that those who distrust the official story of Diana's death do not tend

to settle on a single conspiracist account as the only acceptable explanation; rather, they

simultaneously endorse several contradictory accounts. In Study 2, we set out to conceptually

replicate these findings in another setting, and also to ask why mutually contradictory conspiracy

theories are simultaneously endorsed.

Study 2

On May 2

nd

, 2011, it was reported in the news media that Osama bin Laden had been

killed in an American raid on a compound in Abbottabad, Pakistan. Conspiracy theories alleging

that bin Laden had not actually been killed in the raid immediately started to propagate

throughout the Internet and traditional media, mostly. Proponents claimed that their suspicions

were aroused by several actions of the Obama administration, including a refusal to release

pictures of bin Laden's body and the decision to bury him at sea shortly after the raid.

The conspiracy theories surrounding the death of Osama bin Laden can be divided into two

major categories: those that propose he was already dead at the time of the raid, and those that

propose he is still alive (Kingsley & Jones, 2011). The former seems to have currency among

the 9/11 conspiracist Truth Movement; many "Truthers" allege that bin Laden died in 2000 or

even earlier, and his video appearances since then were in fact staged productions made with a

body double. The latter theory varies; some people believe that he is still at large, while others

think that he was captured alive and is being secretly held for interrogation by the CIA.

Naturally, these two theories are irreconcilable; bin Laden cannot be both alive and dead at the

same time. However, as in Study 1, we predicted that belief in the two conspiracy theories

would be positively correlated.

Further, in order to test the idea that perceived deception by authorities underlies the

positive correlation between contradictory conspiracy theories, we asked participants to what

degree they found the American government's actions surrounding the raid to be suspicious and

indicative of a cover-up. This was intended to operationalize the central principle of

conspiracism outlined above: the idea that authorities are engaged in motivated deception. If

belief in a cover-up is indeed responsible for the positive association between contradictory

conspiracy theories, controlling for it should cause the correlation between the contradictory

theories to disappear.

Method

Participants

One hundred two undergraduate students (58% female, mean age 21) at a British university

were recruited to participate in the study between one and six weeks after the announcement of

bin Laden's death. In exchange for their participation they received a randomized prize of either

a snack or a small monetary reward of GB£1.00 or 2.00 (~US$1.50 or 3.00).

Materials and Procedure

Participants were directed to read a brief summary of the official story of Osama bin

Laden's death, including the details regarding the refusal to release pictorial evidence and the

burial at sea, followed by a short paragraph explaining that some people doubt the official story.

They were then asked about their opinion of the official story, followed by three conspiracy

items:

1. Osama bin Laden was killed in the American raid.

2. Osama bin Laden is still alive.

3. When the raid took place, Osama bin Laden was already dead.

4. The actions of the Obama administration indicate that they are hiding some important

or damaging piece of information about the raid.

Each of these statements was followed by a series of questions based on the composite

endorsement measure used by Douglas and Sutton (2011). This asked participants to rate their

agreement with each statement on a scale from 1 ("strongly disagree") to 6 ("strongly agree"), as

well as to what degree they found the statements plausible, convincing, worth considering, and

coherent, again on a scale from 1 ("not at all") to 6 ("very much"). These ratings were then

averaged to obtain a composite measure of endorsement for each statement (α > .87 for each

statement). While the original measure used by Douglas and Sutton also asked participants to

judge the interestingness of each statement, there is no contradiction in finding two rival theories

equally interesting, so we excluded interestingness from the present study in order to avoid

artificially inflating the relevant correlations.

Results & Discussion

The idea that bin Laden was killed in the raid enjoyed a high level of endorsement (M =

5.00, SD = 1.19), indicating a fairly high level of trust in the official story, though participants on

average found the Obama administration's actions to be suspicious (M = 4.74, SD = 1.41).

Participants seemed less likely to endorse the idea that bin Laden is still alive (M = 3.05, SD =

1.39) or was already dead (M = 3.19, SD = 1.39). In a replication of our Study 1 result, a

correlational analysis revealed a significant positive correlation between composite endorsement

ratings of the two contradictory conspiracy theories, r = .21, p = .04.

We next examined the contribution of belief in a cover-up to the positive relationship

between the two contradictory theories using a hierarchical multiple regression analysis.

Endorsement of the cover-up item significantly predicted endorsement of the "bin Laden is still

alive" theory, β = .373, t(97) = 4.04, p < .001 (the same was true of the already-dead theory, β =

.346, t(97) = 3.63, p < .001). Adding endorsement of the contradictory theory "bin Laden was

already dead" to the regression equation, however, explained no additional variance (R

2

=

.006), and this theory was not itself a significant predictor, β = .086, t(96)= 0.86, p = .40. This

indicates that the correlation in endorsement of the two contradictory theories is explainable

entirely by their connection with belief in a deceptive cover-up by authority (see Figure 1). The

degree to which someone believes in a cover-up helps to determine their endorsement of the

official story, and of both conspiracy theories as well. This result is in line with our predictions,

and supports the idea that conspiracy theories are defined not by adherence to a particular

alternative account, but by opposition to the official story and a belief that deception is taking

place.

General Discussion

While it has been known for some time that belief in one conspiracy theory appears to be

associated with belief in others, only now do we know that this can even apply to conspiracy

theories that are mutually contradictory. This finding supports our contention that the

monological nature of conspiracism (Goertzel, 1994; Swami et al., 2010; Swami et al., 2011) is

driven not by conspiracy theories directly supporting one another, but by the coherence of each

theory with higher-order beliefs that support the idea of conspiracy in general. As demonstrated

in Study 2, perceived deception by authority is one such belief, and it is likely that there are

many others as well. For those who hold such beliefs, the specifics of a conspiracy theory do not

matter as much as the fact that it is a conspiracy theory at all.

There are strong parallels between this conception of a monological belief system and

Adorno's (1954) work on prejudice and authoritarianism. In an attempt to explain the strong

positive correlations between contradictory anti-Semitic beliefs, Adorno suggested that

incompatibilities between beliefs at a local level are dwarfed by coherence with broader beliefs

about the world - "nuclear ideas" which "tend to 'pull in' numerous other opinions and attitudes

and thus to form a broad ideological system." (p. 92). Such a system "provides a rationale for

any specific idea within it and a basis for meeting and assimilating new social conditions" (p.

93). Our findings support an equivalent explanation for beliefs in contradictory conspiracy

theories, with a belief in deceptive officialdom as the nuclear idea in question.

If Adorno's explanation for contradictory anti-Semitic beliefs can indeed be applied to

conspiracy theories, conspiracist beliefs might be most accurately viewed as not only

monological, but also ideological in nature. Just as an orthodox Marxist might interpret major

world events as arising inevitably from the forces of history, a conspiracist would see the same

events as carefully orchestrated steps in a plot for global domination. Conceptualizing

conspiracism as a coherent ideology, rather than as a cluster of beliefs in individual theories, may

be a fruitful approach in the future when examining its connection to ideologically relevant

variables such as social dominance orientation and right-wing authoritarianism.

Although we have demonstrated the importance of a belief in deception by authority as an

important antecedent of conspiracy belief and a partial explanation for correlations between

contradictory theories, there are certainly other broad beliefs which could make a similar

contribution. For instance, conspiracy theories would seem much more plausible to those with a

belief in the effectiveness of intimidation and bribery. In a more abstract sense, a belief in the

essential malevolence of officialdom - or in the specific malevolence of a certain powerful entity

- would make many conspiracies seem more likely. The social element must not be neglected

either; many conspiracy theories are associated with specific groups or even organized

movements, such as the 9/11 Truth Movement. Clarke (2007) found a trend of increasing

vagueness in these modern conspiracist communities, which he characterised as a reaction to the

antagonistic atmosphere of Internet discourse. Our results suggest an alternative possibility: a

genuine uncertainty within individuals regarding the true nature of the conspiracy behind a

particular event (beyond the fact that there was one), and a willingness to consider and even

endorse mutually contradictory accounts as long as they stand in opposition to the officially

sanctioned narrative. There may also be an element of self-presentation and conflict avoidance

in the vagueness observed by Clarke: if multiple contradictory theories are simultaneously

believed by many in a conspiracist community, endorsing one in particular is tantamount to

denying the others, and may provoke a backlash. In any event, the development of conspiracy

theories almost certainly owes a great deal to social engagement and discussion of alternative

narratives, and the dynamics of conspiracist communities may be a fruitful avenue for future

investigation with reference to previous work on opinion-based groups (e.g. Musgrove &

McGarty, 2008).

Conspiracist belief systems may also be well-captured by connectionist models of social

inference such as Thagard's (1989) explanatory coherence model (ECHO). ECHO has been

shown to accurately predict the degree to which higher-order beliefs about social actors affect

judgements of their actions as sinister or innocent, honest or deceptive (Read & Miller, 1993).

However, there has been little or no investigation into the ability of ECHO to model the

influence of broad worldviews. Based on the present research, one would expect that when

broad beliefs are relevant to the interpretation of a particular situation, they serve as a constraint

on the conclusions that are likely to be drawn from it in the same way as specific beliefs about

the actors and situations involved. A conspiracist belief system consisting of many such beliefs

would inhibit the acceptance of official narratives, but may not discriminate among several

different conspiracy theories. Some might be discarded, but even contradictory theories might

be simultaneously accepted. Almost any account of events which accords with the broader

beliefs in question is likely to garner some endorsement by adherents of a conspiracist

worldview. Modelling such a network might provide an instructive insight into the processes

underlying the development of conspiracist beliefs, and of other beliefs influenced by

superordinate ideological considerations.

It must be noted that not all conspiracy theories fall under the "deceptive officialdom"

umbrella. Anti-Semitic conspiracy theories are a notable and historically important exception;

instead of alleging abuse of power by elites, historical theories of Jewish conspiracy usually

detailed supposed attempts by a minority to seize power for themselves (Graumann, 1987). It

would be instructive to examine whether beliefs in such conspiracies are correlated with belief in

those that fit more closely into the "deceptive officialdom" template, and if such relationships are

mediated to the same degree by endorsement of that central belief.

In any case, the evidence we have gathered in the present study supports the idea that

conspiracism constitutes a monological belief system, drawing its coherence from central beliefs

such as the conviction that authorities and officials engage in massive deception of the public to

achieve their malevolent goals. Connectivity with this central idea lends support to any

individual conspiracy theory, even to the point that mutually contradictory theories fail to show a

negative correlation in belief. Believing that Osama bin Laden is still alive is apparently no

obstacle to believing that he has been dead for years.

References:

Bale, J.M. (2007). Political paranoia v. political realism: On distinguishing between bogus

conspiracy theories and genuine conspiratorial politics. Patterns of Prejudice, 41, 45-60.

Clarke, S. (2002). Conspiracy theories and conspiracy theorizing. Philosophy of the Social

Sciences, 32, 131-150.

Clarke, S. (2007). Conspiracy theories and the Internet: Controlled demolition and arrested

development. Episteme: A Journal of Social Epistemology, 4, 167-180

Coady, D. (2006). Conspiracy theories: The philosophical debate. Aldershot: Ashgate

Publishing.

Douglas, K.M., & Sutton, R.M. (2008). The hidden impact of conspiracy theories: Perceived and

actual influence of theories surrounding the death of Princess Diana. Journal of Social

Psychology, 148 , 210-222.

Douglas & Sutton (2011). Does it take one to know one? Endorsement of conspiracy theories is

influenced by personal willingness to conspire. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50,

542-552.

Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15, 731–742.

Goertzel, T. (2010). Conspiracy theories in science. EMBO Reports, 11, 493-499.

Graumann, C.F. (1987). Conspiracy: history and social psychology - a synopsis. In C.F.

Graumann and S. Moscovici (Eds.), Changing conceptions of conspiracy (pp. 245-251).

New York: Springer-Verlag.

Grzesiak-Feldman, M., & Suszek, H. (2008). Conspiracy stereotyping and perceptions of group

entitativity of Jews, Germans, Arabs and homosexuals by Polish students. Psychological

Reports, 102, 755-758.

Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Kay, J. (2011). Among the truthers: A journey through America's conspiracist underground.

New York: HarperCollins.

Inglehart, R. (1987). Extremist political positions and perceptions of conspiracy. In C.F.

Graumann & S. Moscovici (Eds.) Changing conceptions of conspiracy (pp. 231–244). New

York: Springer-Verlag.

Kingsley, P., & Jones, S. (2011, May 5). Osama bin Laden death: The conspiracy theories. The

Guardian. Retrieved from http://www.guardian.co.uk/world/2011/may/05/osama-bin-

laden-conspiracy-theories

Musgrove, L., & McGarty, C. (2008) Opinion-based group membership as a predictor of

collective emotional responses and support for pro- and anti-war action. Social Psychology ,

39, 37-47.

Read, S. J. (1987). Constructing causal scenarios: A knowledge structure approach to causal

reasoning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 288-302.

Read, S. J., & Miller, L.C. (1993). Rapist or "regular guy": Explanatory coherence in the

construction of mental models of others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19,

526-540.

Read, S. J., Snow, C. J., & Simon, D. (2003). Constraint satisfaction processes in social

reasoning. In The Proceedings of the Twenty-fifth Annual Cognitive Science Society

Conference.

Rowe, K. (Producer), Bermas, J. (Producer), Brown, M. (Producer), & Avery, D. (Director)

(2005). Loose Change [Motion Picture]. United States: Microcinema International.

Simon, D., Snow, C.J., & Read, S. J. (2004). The redux of cognitive consistency theories:

Evidence judgments by constraint satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 86, 814-837.

Smith, T.C., & Novella, S.P. (2007). HIV denial in the Internet era. PLoS Medicine, 4, 1312-

1316.

Swami, V., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2010). Unanswered questions: A

preliminary investigation of personality and individual difference predictors of 9/11

conspiracist beliefs. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 749-761.

Swami, V., Coles, R., Stieger, S., Pietschnig, J., Furnham, A., Rehim, S., & Voracek, M. (2011).

Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief system and

associations between individual psychological differences and real-world and fictitious

conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology.

Table 1. Correlations between endorsement of Princess Diana conspiracy theories in Study 1.

Official MI6

campaign to kill

Diana

Dodi and Diana

killed by Al-

Fayeds' business

enemies

Diana had to die

prevent her from

marrying an Arab

Diana faked her

own death

Diana killed by

rogue cell of

British

Intelligence

.749 *** .614 *** .670 *** 0.15

Official MI6

campaign to kill

Diana

1 .660 *** .622 *** .206 *

Dodi and Diana

killed by Al-

Fayeds' business

enemies

1 .607 *** .253 ***

Diana had to die

to prevent her

from marrying an

Arab

1 .242 **

Note: *** p < .001; ** p < .01; * p < .05,

p < .10. Correlations between mutually

contradictory items are bolded. All correlation coefficients are Pearson r.

Figure 1. Illustration of the observed correlations in endorsement of Study 2 items. The two

conspiracy theories display a significant zero-order correlation (above), but have no significant

direct relationship when belief in a cover-up is taken into account (below).

... To have the (epistemic) authority to assert p is to know p." Thus in asserting p, one represents oneself as knowing p is true (Williamson, 2000, p. 252, n. 6). But those who traffic in absurd stories often seem uninterested in knowledge as inconsistencies and contradictions are openly accepted (Lewandowsky, Cook, & Lloyd, 2016;Wood, Douglas, & Sutton, 2012). Even statements within the same conspiracy theory may be openly inconsistent. ...

... In contrast, far-fetched conspiracy theories often have a "self-sealing quality" that renders them irrefutable (Vermeule & Sunstein, 2009;Keeley, 1999;Harris, 2018). Another epistemic shortcoming of conspiracy theorizing is that it allows for strong associations between openly contradictory claims (as in the example Lady Diana above; see Wood et al., 2012). One explanation for this is that such kind of theories may not actually be believed. ...

  • Marianna Bergamaschi Ganapini

Why do people share or publicly engage with fake stories? Two possible answers come to mind: (a) people are deeply irrational and believe these stories to be true; or (b) they intend to deceive their audience. Both answers presuppose the idea that people put the stories forward as true. But I argue that in some cases, these outlandish (yet also very popular) stories function as signals of one's group membership. This signaling function can make better sense of why, despite their unusual nature or lack of a factual basis, some of these stories are so widespread.

... Stąd maksymalnie zwiększona tolerancja dla łamania wszelkich standardów racjonalności, takich jak choćby zasada sprzeczności. Badania pokazują, że jeśli ktoś wierzy w to, że księżna Diana upozorowała swoją śmierć, to najprawdopodobniej równocześnie wierzy w to, że została ona zamordowana przez MI6 (Wood, Douglas, Sutton, 2012). Podobnie jest z fikcyjnymi teoriami wymyślonymi na potrzeby badania: osoby przekonane, że za atakiem na WTC stoi amerykański rząd, o wiele częściej wierzyły w fałszywą informację, że składnik o tajemniczej nazwie testiculus taurus, znajdujący się w napoju Red Bull, ma nieznane skutki uboczne (Swami i in., 2010). ...

  • Szymon Tomasz MakuÅ‚a Szymon Tomasz MakuÅ‚a

Czym jest teoria spiskowa? Wyrażenie "teoria spiskowa" nie posiada jednego powszechnie przyjętego przez naukowców znaczenia, ponieważ autorzy tworzący rozmaite definicje i typologie są badaczami należącymi do odmiennych dyscyplin i patrzą na przedmiot swoich badań z odrębnych perspektyw. Inaczej rozumieją wyrażenie "teoria spiskowa" psychologowie, którzy budują skale myślenia spiskowego, a inaczej filozofowie, którzy zastanawiają się nad ich wiarygod-nością. Termin ten jest również nieostry, a więc rozstrzygnięcie, czy dany zbiór twierdzeń jest teorią spiskową może przysporzyć kłopotów. #0# * Szymon Makuła-dr, adiunkt w Instytucie Filozofii Uniwersytetu Śląskiego. Interesuje się krytycznym myśleniem, teorią argumentacji, logiką nieformalną i meto-dologią nauk.

... Similarly, conspiracy theories have also been associated with highly rigid belief system that are not very permeable to change. Wood, Douglas & Sutton [66] relate conspiracy beliefs to a broad ideologic system, which justifies any belief associated with it, constituting a base ground to understand and assimilate new social conditions. It seems that beliefs in conspiracy theories follow a single-logic approach, through which beliefs on any given theory constitute a way of sustaining other theories [67,68]. ...

  • Simão Pedro Rodrigues Ferreira
  • Carlos Campos Carlos Campos
  • Beatriz Monteiro Marinho
  • Nuno Barbosa-Rocha

Rationale The COVID-19 pandemic is a worldwide threat to public health and the global economy. The climate of fear and uncertainty associated with the pandemic has fostered the emergence of a wide-range of COVID-19 conspiracy theories that has the potential do shape public opinion and hinder the effective dissemination of valid information. Beliefs in conspiracy theories has been associated with maladaptive personality traits such as schizotypy and paranoia, as well as other non-psychotic psychological characteristics (e.g., social isolation, stress). Methods Examined were associations between beliefs in COVID-19 conspiracy theories and psychotic-like experiences within the Portuguese residential community (N = 438), while also addressing the role of sociodemographic information, psychological outcomes (e.g., stress, affective states), confinement-related factors (e.g., confinement conditions/behaviors), and pandemic-related factors (e.g., health concerns, financial issues). Results Findings suggest that psychotic-like experiences are associated with beliefs in COVID-19 conspiracy theories, particularly perceptual abnormalities, and persecutory ideation. Moreover, increased health-related concerns and reduced education levels also seem to be liability factors for these conspiracy beliefs. Conclusion These results add important insights into how the adherence to illogical and erroneous disease-related arguments can be related to psychotic-like experiences. COVID-19 conspiracy theories are yet another major challenge that governments and policy makers must contemplate when defining strategic directions to manage the current and future pandemics.

... While Popper and Hofstadter can be considered as the originators of this literature, the field only began to expand considerably during the 1990s and was particularly influenced by the work of Ted Goertzel (1994), who argued that conspiracy supporters could be characterized by a "monological belief system" that allows people to believe in different conspiracy theories at the same time and to consider such theories as mutually supporting within a general frame of mistrust of mainstream/official versions. Other scholars have contested the idea of a "monological belief system" proposed by Goertzel, basing their criticism on the argument that actually conspiracy theories often contradict each other (Brotherton et al., 2013;Buenting & Taylor, 2010;Lukić et al., 2019;Wood et al., 2012). Goertzel's study was followed by numerous works in the following years that further advanced a pathological understanding of conspiracy theories that emphasized paranoia as a key attribute (see, e.g., Butler et al., 1995;Keeley, 1999;McHoskey, 1995). ...

  • Niccolò Bertuzzi Niccolò Bertuzzi

It is surprising to note the scarcity of contributions in social movement literature related to so-called conspiracy theories. A considerable amount of the work on these topics has been produced in political science, history, media studies, social psychology and other disciplines. These accounts have often adopted a stigmatizing approach, looking at conspiracy theories as forms of pathologies (whether psychological, social or political). Moving from such a perspective to a constructivist one, I argue that conspiracy theories should represent an object of interest for social movement scholars: conspiracies supporters go into the streets to highlight their issues, protest against authority, propose alternative lifestyles and often claim to look for a better/different society. Applying the social movements toolkit can allow to better understand this phenomenon and apply critical perspectives in a more effective manner. On the basis of this premise, the first part of this article reviews the existing literature on conspiracy theories, also identifying the main lacunae; the second part outlines some possible research questions and lines of inquiry, moving beyond the classical theories in the field of social movement studies. The paper also introduces a number of new concepts, such as conspiracy mobilizations and conspiracy coalitions.

... A common and robust finding in the literature is that there is often a tendency toward belief in different conspiracy theories, even when they are contradictory (Wood, Douglas, & Sutton, 2012). In order to understand these associations, it has been theorized that there exists a general and stable attitude toward the acceptance of conspiratorial narratives, called a conspiracy worldview (e.g., Dagnall, Drinkwater, Parker, Denovan, & Parton, 2015), conspiracist ideation (e.g., Brotherton, French, & Pickering, 2013), or conspiracy mentality (Bruder, Haffke, Neave, Nouripanah, & Imhoff, 2013). ...

Previous literature highlights the crucial role of economic inequality in triggering a range of negative societal outcomes. However, the relationship between economic inequality and the proliferation of conspiracy beliefs remains unexplored. Here, we explore the endorsement of conspiracy beliefs as an outcome of objective country-level (Study 1a, 1b, 1c), perceived (Study 2), and manipulated economic inequality (Studies 3a, 3b, 4a, 4b). In the correlational studies, both objective and perceived economic inequality were associated with greater conspiracy beliefs. In the experiments, participants in the high (compared to the low) inequality condition were more likely to endorse conspiratorial narratives. This effect was fully mediated by anomie (Studies 3a, 3b) suggesting that inequality enhances the perception that society is breaking down (anomie), which in turn increases conspiratorial thinking, possibly in an attempt to regain some sense of order and control. Furthermore, the link between economic inequality and conspiracy beliefs was stronger when participants endorsed a conspiracy worldview (Studies 4a, 4b). Moreover, conspiracy beliefs mediated the effect of the economic inequality manipulation on willingness to engage in collective action aimed at addressing economic inequality. The results show that economic inequality and conspiracy beliefs go hand in hand: economic inequality can cause conspiratorial thinking and conspiracy beliefs can motivate collective action against economic inequality.

... A common and robust finding in the literature is that there is often a tendency towards belief in different conspiracy theories, even when they are contradictory (Wood et al., 2012). In order to understand these associations, it has been theorized that there exists a general and stable attitude toward the acceptance of conspiratorial narratives, called a conspiracy worldview (e.g., Dagnall et al., 2015), conspiracist ideation (e.g., Brotherton et al., 2013), or conspiracy mentality (Bruder et al., 2013). ...

Previous literature highlights the crucial role of economic inequality in triggering a range of negative societal outcomes. However, the relationship between economic inequality and the proliferation of conspiracy beliefs remains unexplored. Here, we explore the endorsement of conspiracy beliefs as an outcome of objective country-level (Study 1a, 1b, 1c), perceived (Study 2), and manipulated economic inequality (Studies 3a, 3b, 4a, 4b). In the correlational studies, both objective and perceived economic inequality were associated with greater conspiracy beliefs. In the experiments, participants in the high (compared to the low) inequality condition were more likely to endorse conspiratorial narratives. This effect was fully mediated by anomie (Studies 3a, 3b) suggesting that inequality enhances the perception that society is breaking down (anomie), which in turn increases conspiratorial thinking, possibly in an attempt to regain some sense of order and control. Furthermore, the link between economic inequality and conspiracy beliefs was stronger when participants endorsed a conspiracy worldview (Studies 4a, 4b). Moreover, conspiracy beliefs mediated the effect of the economic inequality manipulation on willingness to engage in collective action aimed at addressing economic inequality. The results show that economic inequality and conspiracy beliefs go hand in hand: economic inequality can cause conspiratorial thinking and conspiracy beliefs can motivate collective action against economic inequality.

  • Konstantinos Fountoulakis Konstantinos Fountoulakis

I decided to write this chapter the last moment. It was September–October of 2020, and the COVID-19 pandemic was at its highest point, or that was what we were thinking at that time, so were conspiracy theories concerning the virus and the outbreak in general. Later this would focus specifically on vaccines. It was more than clear that in the years to come, one of the new topics, if not the real battlegrounds that will occupy the attention of society, will be the conspiracy theories. Psychiatry will likely be involved in this, since the main characteristics of the problem are irrationality, intense emotions including anger, and problematic behaviors that pose dangers to public health and safety, including vaccination denial, following harmful alternative medicine ways, violent acts, and more recently failure to follow the COVID-19 lockdown and safety measures. This new "battlefront" is so new, so uncharted and difficult, and so peculiar that the side of conspiracists includes at least three Nobel laureates, editors of prominent medical journals, and a significant number of reputed scientists, while an additional significant number walks a tightrope between radical unconventional thinking and conspiracism.

  • Gabriel Andrade

Due to complex social, political and economic causes, Venezuela has encountered difficulties managing the Covid-19 pandemic. Although a vaccination program has started, it is still comparatively slow and largely inefficient. One particular challenge has been vaccine hesitancy. Venezuelan policymakers have traditionally not rendered much attention to ethnic disparities. In this study, we assess whether vaccine hesitancy varies across ethnic groups, in a sample of 273 Venezuelan university students. Results come out showing that marginalized ethnic groups in Venezuela are more prone to vaccine hesitancy, and also have greater levels of acceptance regarding conspiracy theories. Coefficients of correlation of ethnic discrimination with vaccine hesitancy and conspiracy beliefs are moderate. Coefficients of correlation of paranoid ideation with vaccine hesitancy and conspiracy beliefs are weak. This suggests that in order to successfully complete the vaccination program, policymakers in Venezuela must begin to approach racial disparities.

  • Vojtech Pisl
  • Jan Volavka
  • Edita Chvojkova
  • Jan Vevera Jan Vevera

Understanding the predictors of the willingness to get vaccinated against COVID-19 may aid in the resolution of current and future pandemics. We investigate how the readiness to believe conspiracy theories and the three dimensions of health locus of control (HLOC) affect the attitude toward vaccination. A cross-sectional study was conducted based on the data from an online survey of a sample of Czech university students (n = 866) collected in January 2021, using the multivariate linear regression models and moderation analysis. The results found that 60% of Czech students wanted to get vaccinated against COVID-19. In addition, 40% of the variance of willingness to get vaccinated was explained by the belief in the COVID-19-related conspiracy theories and the powerful others dimension of HLOC. One-sixth of the variance of the willingness to get vaccinated was explained by HLOC, cognitive reflection, and digital health literacy [eHealth Literacy Scale (EHEALS)]. HLOC and conspiracy mentality (CM) and its predictors are valid predictors of a hesitancy to get vaccinated against COVID-19. The campaigns promoting vaccination should target the groups specifically vulnerable to the conspiracy theories and lacking HLOC related to powerful others.

  • Ted Goertzel Ted Goertzel

Surveyed 348 residents of southwestern New Jersey and found that most believed that several of a list of 10 conspiracy theories were at least probably true. Ss who believed in 1 conspiracy were more likely also to believe in others. Belief in conspiracies was correlated with anomia, lack of interpersonal trust, and insecurity about employment. Blacks and Hispanics were more likely to believe in conspiracy theories than were Whites. Younger Ss were slightly more likely to believe in conspiracy theories, but there were few significant correlations with gender, educational level, or occupational category. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

  • S. Clarke

Following Clarke (2002), a Lakatosian approach is used to account for the epistemic development of conspiracy theories. It is then argued that the hypercritical atmosphere of the internet has slowed down the development of conspiracy theories, discouraging conspiracy theorists from articulating explicit versions of their favoured theories, which could form the hard core of Lakatosian research programmes. The argument is illustrated with a study of the "controlled demolition" theory of the collapse of three towers at the World Trade Center on September 11, 2001.

  • Carl F. Graumann

Social psychologists, when first introduced into the field (or rather, underground world) of conspiracy and conspiratorial conceptions, experience two kinds of surprise. First, they will be struck and then puzzled by the obvious fact that there is a large and complex field of potential social-psychological research of which they have been unaware. Neither in the broad context of the general problems of their field nor in the special domains of the attribution of guilt and blame have they come across conspiracy Not for the first time they will have to concede that there is a topic of intrinsic psychological interest that has been left to history and to other social sciences, as has happened with most social issues that could not easily be tailored into experimental size.

  • Inglehart Ronald Inglehart Ronald

Conspiracy implies secret communication. It occurs when a group is plotting to attain some goal, and keeping their actions secret from those who would otherwise oppose them.

  • Steve Clarke

The dismissive attitude of intellectuals toward conspiracy theorists is considered and given some justification. It is argued that intellectuals are entitled to an attitude of prima facie skepticism toward the theories propounded by conspiracy theorists, because conspiracy theorists have an irrational tendency to continue to believe in conspiracy theories, even when these take on the appearance of forming the core of degenerating research program. It is further argued that the pervasive effect of the "fundamental attribution error" can explain the behavior of such conspiracy theorists. A rival approach due to Brian Keeley, which involves the criticism of a subclass of conspiracy theories on epistemic grounds, is considered and found to be inadequate.

  • Jeffrey Bale Jeffrey Bale

Scholars and intellectuals often fail to pay sufficient attention to the historical and political importance of conspiratorial politics, that is, real-world covert and clandestine activities. This is primarily because they rarely make an effort to distinguish conceptually between such activities, which are a regular if not omnipresent feature of national and international politics, and bogus 'conspiracy theories', elaborate fantasies that purport to show that various sinister, powerful groups with evil intentions, operating behind the scenes, are secretly controlling the course of world events. Bale's purpose is to provide a clear analytical distinction between actual conspiratorial politics and 'conspiracy theories' in the pejorative sense of that term, and to suggest that research methods appropriate to investigating and analysing the former have long been available. In a world full of secret services, surreptitious pressure groups, criminal cartels and terrorist organizations, academics can no longer afford to ignore bona fide conspiratorial activities of various types, which have often had considerable historical significance in the past and are likely to continue to exert an impact on events in the future.

A model is presented of how people construct coherent representations of others. It integrates work on knowledge representations with Kintsch's construction-integration model of discourse comprehension and Thagard's model of explanatory coherence. A major claim is that parallel constraint satisfaction processes, fundamental to connectionist modeling, play a major role in the development of coherent representations. Several topics are examined: (a) the role of making goal inferences in trait inferences, (b) how people combine apparently inconsistent traits to arrive at a coherent impression, and (c) how this parallel process model can account for findings that have been given a serial interpretation in Trope's two-stage model of dispositional inference and Gilbert's work on cognitive busyness. It is argued that this model provides a more parsimonious but broader explanation for attributions than alternatives.